What is the meaning of the term 'motorboating' and what is its origin?

Bot image for Web-Search

The term "motorboating" has two distinct meanings.

The first meaning refers to the activity of traveling in a motorboat [2] . This is the literal definition of the term and is commonly used in the context of recreational boating or water sports.

The second meaning, which is more commonly known as slang, refers to a sexual act. It involves placing one's face between a woman's breasts and making a noise like a motorboat by rapidly moving one's head from side to side [1] [2] . This slang term is often used humorously or in a playful manner.

Origin: The origin of the term "motorboating" in the context of the sexual act is not well-documented. However, it is believed to have emerged as a slang term in popular culture, possibly in the late 20th century or early 21st century. The act itself is a playful and humorous gesture, and the term likely originated as a way to describe the sound and motion made during the act, which resembles the noise and movement of a motorboat.

Learn more:

  • What does motorboating mean? How did the term originate?
  • motorboating - Wiktionary, the free dictionary
  • Where did the term motorboat come from? | AnandTech Forums: Technology, Hardware, Software, and Deals

Continue the conversation

Explore more.

How Electric Motors and Generators Work

Learn How They Generate Power for Electric Cars & Hybrids

Christine and Scott Gable are hybrid auto and alternative fuel experts who have brewed their own biodiesel and traveled 125,000 miles on waste vegetable oil.

  • Millersville University
  • Public Transportation

Electric vehicles rely exclusively on electric motors for propulsion, and hybrids use electric motors to assist their internal combustion engines for locomotion. But that's not all. These very motors can be, and are, used to generate electricity (through the process of regenerative braking ) for charging these vehicles' onboard batteries.

The most common question is: "How can that be ... how does that work?" Most folks understand that a motor is powered by electricity to do work—they see it every day in their household appliances (​washing machines, vacuum cleaners, food processors).

But the idea that a motor can "run backward," actually generating electricity rather than consuming it seems almost like magic. But once the relationship between magnets and electricity (electromagnetism) and the concept of conservation of energy is understood, the mystery disappears.

Electromagnetism

Motor power and electricity generation begin with the property of electromagnetism—the physical relationship between a magnet and electricity. An electromagnet is a device that acts like a magnet, but its magnetic force is manifested and controlled by electricity.

When wire made of conducting material (copper, for example) moves through a magnetic field, current is created in the wire (a rudimentary generator). Conversely, when electricity is passed through a wire that is wound around an iron core, and this core is in the presence of a magnetic field, it will move and twist (a very basic motor).

Motor/Generators

Motor/generators are really one device that can run in two opposite modes. Contrary to what folks sometimes think, that does not mean that the two modes of the motor/generator run backward from each other (that as a motor the device turns in one direction and as a generator, it turns the opposite direction).

The shaft always spins the same way. The "change of direction" is in the flow of electricity. As a motor, it consumes electricity (flows in) to make mechanical power, and as a generator, it consumes mechanical power to produce electricity (flows out).

Electromechanical Rotation

Electric motor/generators are generally one of two types, either AC (Alternating Current) or DC (Direct Current) and those designations are indicative of the type of electricity that they consume and generate.

Without getting into too much detail and clouding the issue, this is the difference: AC current changes direction (alternates) as it flows through a circuit. DC currents flow uni-directionally (stays the same) as it goes through a circuit.

The type of current utilized is concerned mostly with the cost of the unit and its efficiency (An AC motor/generator is generally more expensive, but is also much more efficient). Suffice it to say that most hybrids and many larger all-electric vehicles use AC motor/generators—so that is the type we'll focus on in this explanation.

An AC Motor/Generator Consists of 4 Main Parts:

  • A shaft-mounted wire wound armature (rotor)
  • A field of magnets that induce electrical energy stacked side-by-side in a housing (stator)
  • Slip rings that carry the AC current to/from the armature
  • Brushes that contact the slip rings and transfer current to/from the electrical circuit

The AC Generator in Action

The armature is driven by a mechanical source of power (for example, in commercial electric power production it would be a steam turbine). As this wound rotor spins, its wire coil passes over the permanent magnets in the stator and an electric current is created in the wires of the armature.

But because each individual loop in the coil passes first the north pole then the south pole of each magnet sequentially as it rotates on its axis, the induced current continually, and rapidly, changes direction. Each change of direction is called a cycle, and it is measured in cycles-per-second or hertz (Hz).

In the United States, the cycle rate is 60 Hz (60 times per second), while in most other developed parts of the world it is 50 Hz. Individual slip rings are fitted to each of the two ends of the rotor's wire loop to provide a path for the current to leave the armature. Brushes (which are actually carbon contacts) ride against the slip rings and complete the path for the current into the circuit to which the generator is attached.

The AC Motor in Action

Motor action (supplying mechanical power) is, in essence, the reverse of generator action. Instead of spinning the armature to make electricity, current is fed by a circuit, through the brushes and slip rings and into the armature. This current flowing through the coil wound rotor (armature) turns it into an electromagnet. The permanent magnets in the stator repel this electromagnetic force causing the armature to spin. As long as electricity flows through the circuit, the motor will run.

  • The Two-Mode Hybrid
  • Can You Charge Your Electric Car During a Power Outage?
  • Inverters and Converters in Hybrids and EV (Electric Vehicles)
  • Do Electric Cars Have Transmissions?
  • The History of the Electric Car: A Timeline
  • Ask Pablo: Could a Hacked Hummer H3 Really Get 60 MPG?
  • How Many Electric Cars Are on the Road in the United States?
  • Are Electric Car Batteries Recyclable?
  • Hybrid vs Electric Cars: Which Is Greener?
  • California Dreaming: The Reckoning on Climate
  • Five Facts on Electric Cars
  • Regenerative Braking: How and Why It Works for Electric Cars
  • The Difference Between Plug-In and Standard Hybrid Cars
  • How Does Regenerative Braking Work?
  • Cost of Electric Cars: Affordable Electric Cars and How EVs Can Save You Money
  • Do Electric Cars Use Oil? EV Maintenance Tips

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Published: 01 September 2001

Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action

  • Giacomo Rizzolatti 1 , 2 ,
  • Leonardo Fogassi 1 &
  • Vittorio Gallese 1  

Nature Reviews Neuroscience volume  2 ,  pages 661–670 ( 2001 ) Cite this article

12k Accesses

2184 Citations

97 Altmetric

Metrics details

What are the neural bases of action understanding? Although this capacity could merely involve visual analysis of the action, it has been argued that we actually map this visual information onto its motor representation in our nervous system. Here we discuss evidence for the existence of a system, the 'mirror system', that seems to serve this mapping function in primates and humans, and explore its implications for the understanding and imitation of action.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 12 print issues and online access

176,64 € per year

only 14,72 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on SpringerLink
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

motorboat action meaning

A sensory–motor theory of the neocortex

motorboat action meaning

Delineating visual, auditory and motor regions in the human brain with functional neuroimaging: a BrainMap-based meta-analytic synthesis

motorboat action meaning

Action execution and action observation elicit mirror responses with the same temporal profile in human SII

Gross, C. G., Rocha-Miranda, C. E. & Bender, D. B. Visual properties of neurons in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 35 , 96–111 (1972).

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Tanaka, K., Saito, H. A., Fukada, Y. & Moriya, M. Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 66 , 170–189 (1991).

Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, I. V. “What” and “where” in the human brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4 , 157–165 (1994).

Carey, D. P., Perrett, D. I. & Oram, M. W. in Handbook of Neuropsychology: Action and Cognition Vol. 11 (eds Jeannerod, M. & Grafman, J.) 111–130 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1997).

Google Scholar  

Logothetis, N. Object vision and visual awareness. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8 , 536–544 (1998).

Allison, T., Puce, A. & McCarthy, G. Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4 , 267–278 (2000).

Kanwisher, N. Domain specificity in face perception. Nature Neurosci. 3 , 759–763 (2000).

Merleau-Ponty, M. Phenomenology of Perception (Routledge, London, 1962).

Gallese, V. The “shared manifold” hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy. J. Conscious Stud. 8 , 33–50 (2001).

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119 , 593–609 (1996).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3 , 131–141 (1996).

Rizzolatti, G. et al. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey: II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Exp. Brain Res. 71 , 491–507 (1988).

Murata, A. et al. Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 78 , 2226–2230 (1997).

Rizzolatti G., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. in The Cognitive Neurosciences 2nd edn (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 539–552 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2000).

Perrett, D. I. et al. Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. J. Exp. Biol. 146 , 87–113 (1989).

Perrett, D. I., Mistlin, A. J., Harries, M. H. & Chitty, A. J. in Vision and Action: The Control of Grasping (ed. Goodale, M. A.) 163–342 (Ablex, Norwood, New Jersey, 1990).

Jellema, T. & Perrett, D. I. in Attention & Performance XIX. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action (eds Prinz, W. & Hommel, B.) (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, in the press).

Petrides, M. & Pandya, D. N. Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 228 , 105–116 (1984).

Matelli, M., Camarda, R., Glickstein, M. & Rizzolatti, G. Afferent and efferent projections of the inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 251 , 281–298 (1986).

Cavada, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe. J. Comp. Neurol. 287 , 422–445 (1989).

Seltzer, B. & Pandya, D. N. Parietal, temporal, and occipital projections to cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: a retrograde tracer study. J. Comp. Neurol. 15 , 445–463 (1994).

Article   Google Scholar  

Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G. & Matelli, M. The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106 , 283–296 (1998).

Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Fadiga, L. & Rizzolatti, G. Neurons responding to the sight of goal directed hand/arm actions in the parietal area PF (7b) of the macaque monkey. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 24 , 257 (1998).

Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L. & Rizzolatti, G. in Attention & Performance XIX. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action (eds Prinz, W. & Hommel, B.) 334–355 (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, in the press).

Amaral, D. G. et al. in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Disfunction (ed. Aggleton, J. P.) 1–66 (Wiley-Liss, New York, 1992).

Baron-Cohen, S. Mindblindness: an Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind (MIT Press/Bradford Books, 1995).

Book   Google Scholar  

Adolphs, R. Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3 , 469–479 (1999).

Brothers, L., Ring, B. & Kling, A. Response of neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 41 , 199–213 (1990).

Brothers, L. & Ring, B. A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4 , 107–118 (1992).

Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Ostry, D. & Evans, A. Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. J. Neurosci. 16 , 3737–3744 (1996).

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C. & Lenzi, G. L. Observing and imitating emotion: implications for the neurological correlates of empathy. Proc. First Int. Conf. Soc. Cogn. Neurosci. (2001).

Cole, J. D. About Face (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1999).

Cole, J. D. Empathy needs a face. J. Conscious Stud. 8 , 51–68 (2001).

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons. Arch. Ital. Biol. 137 , 85–100 (1999).

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Gastaut, H. J. & Bert, J. EEG changes during cinematographic presentation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 6 , 433–444 (1954).

Cohen-Seat, G., Gastaut, H., Faure, J. & Heuyer, G. Etudes expérimentales de l'activité nerveuse pendant la projection cinématographique. Rev. Int. Filmol. 5 , 7–64 (1954).

Chatrian, G. E. in Handbook of Electroencephalography (ed. Remond, A.) 104–114 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1976).

Cochin, S., Barthelemy, C., Lejeune, B., Roux, S., & Martineau, J. Perception of motion and qEEG activity in human adults. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 107 , 287–295 (1998).

Cochin, S., Barthelemy, C., Roux, S. & Martineau, J. Observation and execution of movement: similarities demonstrated by quantified electroencephalograpy. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11 , 1839–1842 (1999).

Altschuler, E. L., Vankov, A., Wang, V., Ramachandran, V. S. & Pineda, J. A. Person see, person do: human cortical electrophysiological correlates of monkey see monkey do cell. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 23 , 719 (1997).

Altschuler, E. L. et al. Mu wave blocking by observation of movement and its possible use as a tool to study theory of other minds. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 26 , 68 (2000).

Salmelin, R. & Hari, R. Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic rhythms related to thumb movement. Neuroscience 60 , 537–550 (1994).

Hari, R. & Salmelin, R. Human cortical oscillations: a neuromagnetic view through the skull. Trends Neurosci. 20 , 44–49 (1997).

Salenius, S., Schnitzler, A., Salmelin, R., Jousmaki, V. & Hari, R. Modulation of human cortical rolandic rhythms during natural sensorimotor tasks. Neuroimage 5 , 221–228 (1997).

Hari, R. et al. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95 , 15061–15065 (1998).

Fadiga, L. Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G. & Rizzolatti, G. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73 , 2608–2611 (1995).

Strafella, A. P. & Paus, T. Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport 11 , 2289–2292 (2000).

Baldissera, F., Cavallari, P., Craighero, L. & Fadiga, L. Modulation of spinal excitability during observation of hand actions in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13 , 190–194 (2001).

Rizzolatti, G. et al. Localization of grasp representation in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Exp. Brain Res. 111 , 246–252 (1996).

Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L. & Rizzolatti, G. Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Exp. Brain Res. 112 , 103–111 (1996).

Decety, J. et al. Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain 120 , 1763–1777 (1997).

Grèzes, J., Costes, N. & Decety, J. Top–down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a PET investigation. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 15 , 553–582 (1998).

Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci. 21 , 188–194 (1998).

Von Bonin, G. & Bailey, P. The Neocortex of Macaca Mulatta (Univ. Illinois Press, Urbana, 1947).

Petrides, M. & Pandya, D. N. in Handbook of Neuropsychology Vol. IX (eds Boller, F. & Grafman, J.) 17–58 (Elsevier, New York, 1997).

Krams, M., Rushworth, M. F., Deiber, M. P., Frackowiak, R. S. & Passingham, R. E. The preparation, execution and suppression of copied movements in the human brain. Exp. Brain Res. 120 , 386–398 (1998).

Binkofski, F. et al. A fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11 , 3276–3286 (1999).

Ehrsson, H. H. et al. Cortical activity in precision- versus power-grip tasks: an fMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 83 , 528–536 (2000).

Iacoboni, M. et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286 , 2526–2528 (1999).

Nishitani, N. & Hari, R. Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97 , 913–918 (2000).

Buccino, G. et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13 , 400–404 (2001).

Jellema, T., Baker, C. I., Wicker, B. & Perrett, D. I. Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. Brain Cogn 44 , 280–302 (2000).

Umiltà, M. A. et al. “I know what you are doing”: a neurophysiological study. Neuron 32 , 91–101 (2001).

Assad, J. A. & Maunsell, J. H. R. Neuronal correlates of inferred motion in primates posterior parietal cortex. Nature 373 , 518–521 (1995).

Fillion, C. M., Washburn, D. A. & Gulledge, J. P. Can monkeys ( Macaca mulatta ) represent invisible displacement? J. Comp. Psychol. 110 , 386–395 (1996).

Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. in “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes (eds Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R.) 247–273 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1990).

Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. in Imitation in Animals and Artifacts (eds Dautenhahn, K. & Nehaniv, C.) (MIT Press, Boston, Massachusetts, in the press).

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. in The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution and Brain Bases (eds Prinz, W. & Meltzoff, A.) (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, in the press).

Spence, K. W. Experimental studies of learning and higher mental processes in infra-human primates. Psychol. Bull. 34 , 806–850 (1937).

Thorpe, W. H. Learning and Instinct in Animals 2nd edn (Methuen and Co. Ltd, London, 1963).

Whiten, A. & Ham, R. On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. Adv. Study Behav. 21 , 239–283 (1992).

Whiten, A. Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 112 , 270–281 (1998).

Tomasello, M. & Call, J. Primate Cognition (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1997).

Byrne, R. W. The Thinking Ape. Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995).

Tinbergen, N. The Herring Gull's World (Collins, London, 1953).

Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198 , 75–78 (1977).

Bråten, S. (ed.) Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1999).

Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (J. Murray, London, 1872).

Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M. & Elmehed, K. Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 11 , 86–89 (2000).

Hepp-Reymond, M. C., Hüsler, E. J., Maier, M. A. & Qi, H.-X. Force-related neuronal activity in two regions of the primate ventral premotor cortex. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 72 , 571–579 (1994).

Fogassi, L. et al. Visual responses in the dorsal premotor area F2 of the macaque monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 128 , 194–199 (1999).

Gentilucci, M. et al. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. I. Somatotopy and the control of proximal movements. Exp. Brain Res. 71 , 475–490 (1988).

Hoshi, E. & Tanji, J. Integration of target and body-part information in the premotor cortex when planning action. Nature 408 , 466–470 (2000).

Byrne, R. Imitation in action. Adv. Study Behav. (in the press).

Byrne, R. W. Imitation without intentionality: using string-parsing to copy the organization of behaviour. Anim. Cogn 2 , 63–72 (1999).

Hikosaka, O., Rand, M. K., Miyachi, S. & Miyashita, K. Learning of sequential movements in the monkey: process of learning and retention of memory. J. Neurophysiol. 74 , 1652–1661 (1995).

Hikosaka, O., Miyashita, K., Miyachi, S., Sakai, K. & Lu, X. Differential roles of the frontal cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum in visuomotor sequence learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 70 , 137–149 (1998).

Hikosaka, O. et al. in The Cognitive Neurosciences 2nd edn (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 553–572 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2000).

Tanji, J. New concepts of the supplementary motor area. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6 , 782–787 (1996).

Tanji, J., Shima, K. & Mushiake, H. Multiple cortical motor areas and temporal sequencing of movements. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 5 , 117–122 (1996).

Shima, K. & Tanji, J. Neuronal activity in the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas for temporal organization of multiple movements. J. Neurophysiol. 84 , 2148–2160 (2000).

Wolpert, D. M. Computational approaches to motor control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1 , 209–216 (1997).

Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Jordan, M. I. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269 , 1880–1882 (1995).

Kawato, M. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Neuroreport 9 , 718–727 (1999).

CAS   Google Scholar  

Arbib, M. E. & Rizzolatti, G. in The Nature of Concepts. Evolution, Structure, and Representation (ed. Van Loocke, P.) 128–154 (Routledge, London, 1999).

Greenwald, A. G. Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychol. Rev. 77 , 73–99 (1970).

Prinz, W. Perception and action planning. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 9 , 129–154 (1997).

Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A. & Prinz, W. Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial and imitative cues. Brain Cogn 44 , 124–143 (2000).

Iacoboni, M. et al. Mirror properties in a sulcus angularis area. Neuroimage 5 , S821 (2000).

Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12 , 493–501 (1998).

Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. Interacting minds: a biological basis. Science 286 , 1692–1695 (1999).

Blakemore, S.-J. & Decety, J. From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2 , 561–567 (2001).

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T. & Perrett, D. I. Imitation, mirror neurons, and autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25 , 287–295 (2001).

Von Economo, C. The Cytoarchitectonics of the Human Cerebral Cortex (Oxford Univ. Press, London, 1929).

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

the Istituto di Fisiologia Umana, Università di Parma, Via Volturno 39, Parma, I-43100, Italy

Giacomo Rizzolatti, Leonardo Fogassi & Vittorio Gallese

Giacomo Rizzolatti

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Related links

Mit encyclopedia of cognitive sciences.

Positron emission tomography

Motor control

Magnetic resonance imaging

Attribution theory

Perception of motion

Theory of mind

A variant of the transcranial magnetic stimulation technique, in which two coils are used to generate magnetic fields in quick succession over the same cortical region or in different regions at the same time.

Also known as the Hoffmann reflex, the H reflex results from the stimulation of sensory fibres, which causes an excitatory potential in the motor neuron pool after a synaptic delay. Exceeding the potential threshold for a given motor neuron generates an action potential. The resulting discharge will cause the muscle fibres innervated by that neurone to be activated.

A movement not directed towards an object.

A disorder characterized by facial paralysis, attributed to defects in the development of the sixth (abducens) and seventh (facial) cranial nerves.

A philosophical movement founded by the German Edward Husserl, dedicated to describing the structures of experience as they present themselves to consciousness, without recourse to theory, deduction or assumptions from other disciplines, such as the natural sciences.

Stimuli devised by the Swedish psychologist Johannson to study biological motion without interference from shape. Light sources are attached to the joints of people and their movements are recorded in a dark environment.

A technique used to stimulate relatively restricted areas of the human cerebral cortex. It is based on the generation of a strong magnetic field near the area of interest which, if changed rapidly enough, will induce an electric field sufficient to stimulate neurons.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci 2 , 661–670 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1038/35090060

Download citation

Published : 01 September 2001

Issue Date : 01 September 2001

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/35090060

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Cognitive impacts of group dynamics: exploring memory errors in observing actions by familiar and unfamiliar individuals.

  • Lijuan Wang

Current Psychology (2024)

Ultra-high field fMRI identifies an action-observation network in the common marmoset

  • Alessandro Zanini
  • Audrey Dureux
  • Stefan Everling

Communications Biology (2023)

Facilitation of imitative movement in patients with chronic hemiplegia triggered by illusory ownership

  • Tamami Sudo
  • Shin-Ichi Izumi

Scientific Reports (2023)

Simultaneous multimodal fNIRS-EEG recordings reveal new insights in neural activity during motor execution, observation, and imagery

  • Wan-Chun Su
  • Hadis Dashtestani
  • Amir Gandjbakhche

Structural and functional neural substrates underlying the concreteness effect

Brain Structure and Function (2023)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

motorboat action meaning

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2024 Jan-.

Cover of StatPearls

StatPearls [Internet].

Supreeth N. Gowda ; Brendan Hodis ; Lynne Kolton Schneider .

Affiliations

Last Update: May 16, 2024 .

  • Continuing Education Activity

Apraxia is a neurological disorder that affects motor cognition, planning, and task performance without apparent neurological insult to basic motor function, sensation, or comprehension. This disorder can result from various types of brain lesions, including stroke, dementia, tumors, neurocognitive disorders, and brain injuries. Identifying and diagnosing apraxia primarily relies on clinical assessment, lacking broad consensus on methods and definitions. Understanding the relevant neuroanatomy and conducting a comprehensive physical examination are crucial for evaluating and managing the condition. Treatment modalities involve managing the underlying disorder and implementing associated supportive measures. Early identification of apraxia can significantly enhance the patient's ability to perform activities of daily living.

This activity comprehensively reviews the etiology, pathophysiology, clinical assessment, evaluation, treatment, and complications of apraxia. This activity also emphasizes the integral role of the interprofessional healthcare team in evaluating and treating patients with this condition. This activity offers strategies for fostering effective communication and collaboration among multidisciplinary healthcare team members involved in apraxia care, thereby optimizing patient outcomes. 

  • Identify the signs and symptoms of apraxia across diverse patient populations and clinical settings.
  • Implement evidence-based interventions tailored to the specific needs and abilities of patients with apraxia.
  • Select appropriate assistive devices and technologies to support patients with apraxia in their daily activities.
  • Collaborate with occupational therapists, speech-language pathologists, and other healthcare professionals to provide comprehensive care for patients with apraxia.
  • Introduction

Apraxia is the inability to perform skilled movements, whether they have been previously learned or can be immediately imitated by observing or carrying out the instructions of another individual. [1] [2] Diagnosis involves ruling out weakness, sensory dysfunction, comprehension deficits, or incoordination as potential causes. [1] [2] [3] Initially, Hugo Karl Liepmann classified apraxia into 3 types—limb-kinetic, ideomotor, and ideational. [4] Liepmann's descriptions laid the groundwork for today's characterizations. However, the concept of apraxia now comprises a broader spectrum of subtypes compared to those originally identified by Liepmann. Broadly, apraxia can be classified based on specific tasks performed or general actions taken. [5] Unfortunately, the subtypes of apraxia are not defined consistently in the literature. [6]

Major forms of apraxia are listed below.

  • Ideational: Loss of neural encoding of the concept of a previously known skill
  • Ideomotor: Impaired connection between the concept of a skill and its motor output
  • Limb-kinetic: Loss of the motor output associated with a given skill
  • Conceptual: Loss of ability to use tools
  • Specific constructional: Difficulty in drawing, constructing, or copying

The term "ideational apraxia" is sometimes alternatively used to describe the loss of the ability to perform a sequence of movements. In contrast, "conceptual apraxia" refers to the loss of the concept itself. [2] [6]  However, a thorough discussion of usage differences is beyond the scope of this article.

Praxis, or the ability to carry out skilled actions, involves the activation or inhibition of neural networks in the brain. The type of apraxia manifested can vary depending on the involved neural network. [3]  Apraxia can be diagnosed by performing a comprehensive examination on patients, which includes a detailed history, neurological examination, and apraxia-specific testing. However, consensus on the best operational practices for assessing apraxia does not exist. [1]

The management of apraxia involves addressing its underlying causes through physical, occupational, or other task-specific therapies, alongside counseling. Long-term outcomes depend on the type of apraxia and its effect on the patient's activities of daily living. Associated deficits can vary from acalculia, agraphia, and aphasia to confusion, social anxiety, and low self-esteem. Some individuals with apraxia may necessitate long-term assisted nursing care.

Some of the known causes of apraxia include:

  • Corticobasal syndrome  [8]
  • Alzheimer disease  [9]
  • Huntington disease
  • Multiple sclerosis  [10]
  • Tumors  [11]
  • Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease  [12]
  • Schizophrenia
  • Traumatic brain injury  [13]

The risk of developing a specific form of apraxia depends on the risk of developing its associated etiological disorder.

  • Epidemiology

Apraxia is prevalent in approximately 50% to 80% of individuals with left hemisphere strokes, 30% to 50% with right hemisphere strokes, 19% to 45% with traumatic brain injuries, 25% with multiple sclerosis, and 90% with dementia. [14] [15]  However, population-level incidence and prevalence data for apraxia are unfortunately limited.

  • Pathophysiology

Praxis has various conceptual subdivisions. One example is its subdivision into the use of objects or tools and the performance of gestures, either by imitation or on command. [16] Although the neural networks subserving different types of praxis are nonidentical, specific brain structures, such as the left inferior parietal lobule and frontal motor areas, are involved in praxis and apraxias. [16] [17] [18] [19]

A typical schema for the structures involved in praxis illustrates that praxis networks can be activated through visual, auditory, verbal, or tactile stimuli. The activated sensory modality provides information to neurons in the left parietal lobe, which selectively fires to commence the praxis process. [20] [21]  Subsequently, there is feedforward to the supplementary and premotor areas, where corresponding maps of coordinated movements are accessed. This information is sent to the primary motor cortex, which initiates the encoded motor programs by activating the musculature through the pyramidal tracts. [2]

Models of praxis and its associated networks are continually evolving. Additional structures are implicated, including the thalami, basal ganglia, prefrontal regions, temporal regions, and connecting white matter. Damage to these structures, which constitute the praxis networks, can result in apraxia, depending on the role of the damaged structure within its network. [2]

  • History and Physical

Evaluation of apraxia can begin after a comprehensive neurological examination, which excludes sensory, motor, and cognitive dysfunction as potential causes of the observed deficits. Before testing, the patient must first demonstrate an understanding of and ability to execute the tasks used for testing. Although there is no prescribed sequence for testing limbs, both sides should undergo evaluation. [2] [22]

In general, 2 types of actions are assessed—those that are directly imitated and those that are recalled from memory. These actions can be further subdivided into 3 categories—intransitive gestures, transitive gestures, and pantomime of tool use. Intransitive gestures, also known as symbolic gestures, do not depend on using an object, such as saluting or waving goodbye. Transitive gestures involve using an object, such as swinging a hammer or flipping a coin. Pantomime involves mimicking or simulating the use of tools or objects without them being physically present. In addition, meaningless gestures, such as holding the thumb to the underside of the nose, can also be assessed. [2] [22]

Examples of abnormal findings associated with apraxia subtypes are listed below. [2]

  • Ideomotor apraxia: Patients exhibit an inability to pantomime or imitate gestures. Moreover, they experience challenges with spatiotemporal orientation and positioning. Movements involving their fingers, hands, and arms display abnormal trajectories. 
  • Ideational apraxia: Patients struggle to identify the correct sequence of actions necessary to accomplish a task. Even when provided with a list of required steps, they may encounter difficulties organizing them accurately.
  • Limb-kinetic apraxia: Patients exhibit incorrect fine motor actions, particularly with their hands, when attempting to perform a learned task. For instance, they may struggle with rotating a coin using their thumb, index finger, and middle finger.
  • Conceptual apraxia: Patients face challenges identifying the appropriate tool for a given task. When presented with a tool, they may struggle to discern its purpose. Additionally, they may experience difficulty in pantomiming the correct utilization of a tool.

Formal testing tools for limb apraxia include the Florida Apraxia Battery–Extended and Revised Sydney (FABERS), the Apraxia Battery for Adults-2, the Short Screening Test for Ideomotor Apraxia (STIMA), the Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS) or Revised Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS-R), the Diagnostic Instrument for Limb Apraxia (DILA) or the Diagnostic Instrument for Limb Apraxia-Short Version (DILA-S), and the Test of Upper Limb Apraxia (TULIA). [23] [24] [25] [26] [27]

Thorough assessment methods, including detailed history-taking, physical examination, and neuropsychological testing, are used to identify and classify apraxia. Additional testing is used not to describe the apraxia further but to determine its etiology. Apraxia may develop acutely after a neurological insult such as stroke or traumatic brain injury. Alternatively, it may also be insidious, as observed in neurodegenerative disorders. Patients are often unaware of their inability to perform previously learned skills and activities.

Radiological and laboratory evaluation of apraxia can assist in uncovering etiologies associated with apraxia, including cerebrovascular disease, neurodegenerative disorders, traumatic brain injury, tumors, and multiple sclerosis. Imaging modalities such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) can identify associated brain lesions.

  • Treatment / Management

The primary treatment for apraxia involves focused rehabilitation utilizing various therapies such as occupational, speech, and physical, alongside addressing the underlying disorder. Apraxia can significantly impair independent functional capacity, with its presence often indicating the level of caregiver assistance needed post-stroke, while its absence may predict an early return to work. [28]

Currently, a widely accepted treatment strategy for apraxia does not exist. The challenge of creating task-specific therapies with generalizable benefits is illustrated in the work of Buxbaum et al. [29] Rehabilitation is ideally initiated promptly following apraxia diagnosis, particularly in cases stemming from acute lesions. [30] [31] Newer technologies, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation, have shown promise in treating apraxia. [32]

  • Differential Diagnosis

Several disorders related to movement or speech can be mistaken for apraxia, such as aphasia, nonapraxic dysarthria, alien limb phenomenon, akinesia, magnetic grasp, grope reflex, motor preservation, and motor impersistence.

Patients with apraxia can have significant issues with performing learned skills, which can profoundly affect their independence and ability to carry out activities of daily living. Appropriate safety precautions must be implemented with objects and activities in their surroundings that can cause potential injury. In cases where the inability to perform certain learned skills is debilitating, patients may require skilled nursing care. The eventual prognosis varies depending on the patient and the underlying etiological disease. 

  • Complications

Potential sequelae of apraxia include:

  • Inability to perform activities of daily living
  • Failure to function properly at work
  • Injury caused by improper tool use
  • Social isolation
  • Social anxiety
  • Low self-esteem
  • Deterrence and Patient Education

The most common causes of apraxia include stroke, dementia, and traumatic brain injury. Apraxia is often a sign of a complex underlying neurological condition that poses challenges in its management. Education regarding modifiable risk factors should be prioritized whenever possible. Pharmacological interventions and lifestyle management addressing hypertension, diabetes mellitus, sleep apnea, atrial fibrillation, tobacco use, and alcohol consumption can contribute to better outcomes.

Regardless of the underlying causes, the inability to perform specific tasks due to apraxia can evoke frustration, potentially resulting in loss of capacity, diminished motivation, depression, and further deterioration of function. Educational resources, effective communication with caregivers, and counseling of family members can be crucial in establishing realistic expectations, understanding care goals, and anticipating the challenges in the recovery journey. Active participation by patients, caregivers, and family members can significantly facilitate the rehabilitation process.

  • Pearls and Other Issues

Key facts to keep in mind about apraxia include:

  • Apraxia is a neurological disorder characterized by the inability to perform learned or purposeful movements.
  • Types of apraxia include ideomotor, ideational, limb-kinetic, and conceptual forms.
  • Common causes of apraxia include stroke, traumatic brain injury, dementia, neurodegenerative disorders, tumors, and schizophrenia.
  • Although occupational, physical, and speech therapy may help improve functional impairments caused by apraxia, addressing the underlying cause is necessary.
  • The prognosis depends on the underlying cause and promptness of intervention.
  • Enhancing Healthcare Team Outcomes

Apraxia poses a challenge for clinicians due to the complexity of its identification and testing. The presence of concurrent neurological and psychological factors can obscure symptoms. Causes of apraxia range from acute disorders such as stroke and traumatic brain injury to chronic conditions such as neurodegenerative illness or schizophrenia. Furthermore, it can be mistaken for neurological conditions such as aphasia, abulia, and limb akinesia. 

The lack of broad consensus concerning screening and diagnosis can frustrate healthcare providers. However, a detailed history, physical examination, and targeted testing can facilitate early identification and management. Despite inconsistencies in subtype definitions, healthcare professionals prioritize a multidisciplinary approach to diagnosing and managing apraxia, emphasizing seamless communication and collaboration within the healthcare team, thereby gaining valuable insights into the complexities of diagnosing and managing the condition.

The involvement of multidisciplinary and interprofessional teams is paramount in managing apraxia to enhance patient outcomes. This team can include the patient, their family members, nurses, occupational therapists, physiotherapists, primary care physicians, radiologists, neurologists, physiatrists, psychiatrists, counselors, social workers, and case workers. Each healthcare team member should maintain open communication channels with the rest of the team and be responsible for maintaining accurate and detailed records of interactions, interventions, and testing. Such an interprofessional approach significantly enhances patient outcomes.

In both inpatient and outpatient settings, a primary care physician or neurologist can assess for apraxia through comprehensive examination and history-taking. Nurses caring for patients can identify deficiencies in limb function, tool usage knowledge, and general demeanor alterations. Family members can aid in identifying patient weaknesses while they undergo rehabilitation. Therapists can assist in improving limb function and help patients acquire new skills for limb and tool usage. Psychological support during the recovery and rehabilitation phases is vital for long-term patient well-being. 

Establishing a supportive environment at home and work for the patient's condition can facilitate their recovery process. The long-term outcome depends on the etiology and duration of the illness. However, prompt identification and management of the condition by an interprofessional healthcare team can lead to an improved prognosis.

  • Review Questions
  • Access free multiple choice questions on this topic.
  • Comment on this article.

Disclosure: Supreeth Gowda declares no relevant financial relationships with ineligible companies.

Disclosure: Brendan Hodis declares no relevant financial relationships with ineligible companies.

Disclosure: Lynne Kolton Schneider declares no relevant financial relationships with ineligible companies.

This book is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0) ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ ), which permits others to distribute the work, provided that the article is not altered or used commercially. You are not required to obtain permission to distribute this article, provided that you credit the author and journal.

  • Cite this Page Gowda SN, Hodis B, Kolton Schneider L. Apraxia. [Updated 2024 May 16]. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2024 Jan-.

In this Page

Bulk download.

  • Bulk download StatPearls data from FTP

Related information

  • PMC PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed Links to PubMed

Similar articles in PubMed

  • Review Apraxia: Review and Update. [J Clin Neurol. 2017] Review Apraxia: Review and Update. Park JE. J Clin Neurol. 2017 Oct; 13(4):317-324.
  • Callosal apraxia. [Brain. 1983] Callosal apraxia. Watson RT, Heilman KM. Brain. 1983 Jun; 106 (Pt 2):391-403.
  • Review Limb apraxias: higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. [Brain. 2000] Review Limb apraxias: higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. Leiguarda RC, Marsden CD. Brain. 2000 May; 123 ( Pt 5):860-79.
  • Review Hugo Liepmann, Parkinson's disease and upper limb apraxia. [Cortex. 2020] Review Hugo Liepmann, Parkinson's disease and upper limb apraxia. Heilman KM. Cortex. 2020 Oct; 131:79-86. Epub 2020 Jul 21.
  • Review Apraxia: neural mechanisms and functional recovery. [Handb Clin Neurol. 2013] Review Apraxia: neural mechanisms and functional recovery. Foundas AL. Handb Clin Neurol. 2013; 110:335-45.

Recent Activity

  • Apraxia - StatPearls Apraxia - StatPearls

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

  • A-Z of motor boats: your ultimate guide

Motor boats don't often take centre stage in our magazine, but we're about to change that. This in-depth feature explores the different kinds of motorboats, their manufacturers, how they differ from sailboats, and weighs their pros and cons. We'll help you figure out if a motorboat is the right fit for you, when to best venture out on one, and we'll delve into the requirements and conditions for a skipper's licence. In essence, we're bringing you the complete motorboat rundown. All hail the engine!

Differences to a sailboat

The age-old debate of powerboat versus sailboat is a classic theme in many a nautical conversation. We're not here to pick a side between those who favour sails and those who prefer motors. Instead, our aim is to present you with a balanced view, packing all the necessary facts, insights, and knowledge into one comprehensive discussion.

Draft and bridges

A motorboat's draft is significantly shallower, thanks to the absence of a keel. Furthermore, the lack of a mast means there's no need to worry about the boat's height when it comes to passing under bridges. So from a depth and overhead clearance perspective, you're in safe waters with a motorboat.

YACHTING.COM TIP: If you've never sailed under the renowned Pasman-Ugljan bridge, which has spelled disaster for numerous sailing boats, a motorboat provides the perfect chance!

Space and comfort

Broadly speaking, aside from mega yachts or specialist vessels, motorboats provide more space both below and on deck compared to similarly sized sailboats. They also typically feature multiple deck levels. So you can bask in the sunshine on one deck, and find shelter in the shade on another. Furthermore, on a motorboat, you don't have to fret about a precarious jib or the risk of tripping over winches or ropes. The deck tends to be more open and free from sailing gear, allowing for easier movement and relaxation.

If you have crew members who do not tolerate the heeling of a sailboat well, this concern is completely eliminated with motor boats. Unless you're faced with sizeable waves, the boat is likely to maintain stability and you won't need to worry about any significant tilting. This makes a motorboat a more comfortable choice for those sensitive to the motion of the sea.

You wouldn't find such a load on a motorboat

A leisure sailboat simply can't match the speed of a powerboat. While most sailboats average around 7 knots, motorboats can easily reach 15 to 20 knots. If you enjoy the thrill of speed and the feeling of wind in your hair, a powerboat is the perfect choice for you.

Consumption and costs

On the flip side, with the increased speed comes higher fuel costs. While on a sailboat, you might only need to refuel at the end of your trip or 2-3 times a week at most, resulting in a manageable fuel bill. However, if you're sailing for extended periods each day on a motorboat, you'll find yourself refuelling frequently, at a higher cost, and spending a significant amount of time waiting to fill up the diesel tank.

Level of effort and work

Starting a motorboat is straightforward; turn it on and off you go, cruising wherever you fancy. There's no need to fuss over ropes, the jib, sails, lazy bags, lazy jacks, or the whereabouts of the crank. Unlike on a sailboat where there's always something to keep you occupied, a motorboat offers pure relaxation and peace of mind. If you're seeking a laid-back cruising experience, a powerboat is the way to go.

Sailing direction

As long as there are no big waves and the Bora is not blowing against you, you can sail your motorboat comfortably pretty much anywhere you want. This isn't the case with sailboats, where you might have to cruise or alter your destination if the wind is blowing directly against you. While sailboat enthusiasts often say, "the journey is the destination," powerboat users are more about reaching their destination promptly and without fuss.

What is the difference between a motor boat and a sailboat?

Despite their differences, powerboats and sailboats do share some commonalities, with maintenance being the prime one. Regardless of the type of boat you own, upkeep is crucial. This includes taking care of the sails or engine and ensuring regular servicing. Moreover, marina fees apply uniformly to both. The harbour masters charge based on the length of the boat, irrespective of whether it's a sailboat or a powerboat. The only exception might be a catamaran, which typically incurs a higher fee due to its dual-hulled design, making it wider and potentially occupying the space of two conventional berths.

Disadvantages of motor boats

While motor boats offer numerous advantages, it's important to consider their potential drawbacks as well. Let's take off the rose-tinted glasses and delve into some of the downsides associated with powerboats.

Fuel dependency and non-environmental operation

Unlike a sailboat that can harness the wind as a natural and free power source, a motorboat is completely reliant on diesel fuel. Running out of fuel in the middle of your journey can leave you stranded. Furthermore, this dependence on fossil fuels also means that operating a motorboat has a greater environmental impact compared to sailing.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Speaking of ecology, check out our guide — Green sailing: 11 tips for eco-friendly yachting . 

Less stability in wind

Motorboats lack a significant keel, resulting in reduced stability when faced with waves and strong winds. Consequently, it is advisable to opt for motorboat rentals during the summer season, when occurrences of powerful winds and waves are comparatively infrequent.

Calm and the smell of the sea

The sound of the engine never leaves you during your voyage which can get on people's nerves. Likewise, the typical smell of burning diesel can start to bother you after a while.

Who is a motor boat best suited for?

A motor boat is well-suited for individuals seeking relaxation, tranquillity, and minimal effort. With the simple act of starting the engine, you can swiftly set sail without any additional concerns. Plus, a motor boat is highly recommended for those who desire to explore a wide range of places, including beaches and other scenic locations. It is particularly advantageous for covering long distances between islands and the mainland within the typical timeframe of a one or two-week vacation. Motor yachts are also a favourable choice for yachters who enjoy fishing, as they provide a comfortable and convenient means of transportation for navigating to different areas and indulging in fishing activities.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Find out what else you can do while sailing in our article — Top 12 fun activities to do on a sailing holiday .

Fishing is an great addition to a boating holiday.

For nature lovers seeking harmony and a closer connection to the natural environment, a sailboat is more preferable than a motorboat. Sailboats provide a serene atmosphere and allow for a deeper appreciation of nature. Additionally, if the aim is to foster teamwork and engage in shared experiences, a sailboat offers more opportunities as it involves handling ropes and sails.  But if you want to relax with a bunch of friends, there's nothing better than a powerboat.

Motor boat season

Unlike sailing boats that typically operate in Europe from April to November, motor boats have a more limited season. The majority of motor cruising occurs between June and September, with peak activity in June and July. Other times of the year, motor yachts are less commonly seen at sea. This is because before and after this season, conditions tend to be windier and the sea becomes cooler, which is more appealing to racers on sailing yachts rather than those seeking a tranquillity on a motorboat, particularly in destinations like Croatia.

YACHTING.COM TIP: What winds and weather will you encounter in the Mediterranean over summer? Check out our guide — The 7 most common winds you'll find in the Mediterranean . 

Motor boat licence

The licence needed to operate a motor boat depends on two criteria — the engine power and the area where you will be boating (whether sea or inland waters). If you want to cruise on a motor boat with an engine power of  less than 4kW , then you don't need a licence. This applies to houseboats or small boats, for example. You can sail a boat with a 4kW to 20kW  engine on inland waters with a VMP licence, but for the sea you'll need an international skipper's licence just as for a sailing boat and in some countries (such as Croatia), a radio licence. With engine power  above 20kW , for inland sailing and on the sea, you will need a certificate of engine experience for inland sailing in addition to the VMP.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Still hesitating about getting your skipper's licence? Take a look at our 5 reasons to take a skipper's course . Then check out our sailing courses and you'll soon be sailing the seas!

How to choose a motor boat?

Motor boats have a slightly different interior layout than sailboats. The smaller ones often have only one or two cabins and it is automatically assumed that the other couple sleeps in the saloon, often in the bow. Check before you make your final booking that you will have plenty of privacy. Small motor boats are designed for a couple or small family rather than several people who don't know each other.

Route planning

When choosing a boat, take note of how much the boat consumes. You may find that the fuel will cost you the same amount of money as the charter itself in a week's sailing. Plan your itinerary in advance so you know what to expect.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Want to enjoy your cruise to the fullest and without a care in the world? Try hiring a professional skipper or hostess for your yacht. They'll take care of running the boat, cleaning and cooking, leaving you to relax and spend time with your loved ones. Just ask our sales team.

Highly renowned motor boat brands in the charter industry

Here we have picked out the most popular types of motor boats from our search portal.

Probably the most infamous brand of motorboats is Merry Fisher. The Merry Fisher 795 models are among the best sellers and the Merry Fisher 895 is a common sight cruising the coastline of Croatia. Another sought-after model is the Antares 9 OB , which is generously equipped for a comfortable boating holiday, but if you're after something bigger, the Antares 11 Fly is a great choice. The Greenline 33 or its larger sibling, the Greenline 39 , are also fantastic options.

The popular Antares 9 OB model.

Other types of motor boats

Every motor boat is unique, and there can be a wide range of vessels categorized under the name "motor boat." Let's explore some intriguing and lesser-known motor boats that have distinctive features and stand out from the norm.

Small motorboat

Charter services also offer the option to rent small motor boats, which are perfect for day trips to secluded beaches, nearby islands, or bays that are inaccessible by foot. These boats are typically compact and may not have cabins, making them suitable for short excursions. They are particularly recommended for families who have rented an apartment by the sea and wish to explore the surrounding areas by water. In many cases, these small motor boats are equipped with relatively low-powered engines, and in several countries, you may not even require a skipper's license to operate them. We recommend, for example, the Zodiac Madline 2 or the slightly larger Four Winns H210 .

You can also rent a smaller boat.

Few people can buy a superyacht. And although many more people can rent one, it is still quite expensive. A superyacht or megayacht is considered to be a boat longer than 80 feet but you'll have to hire a professional skipper as only a handful of skippers have a licence for a boat of this length. For example, we offer the superyacht Azimut Grande 27 or MY Custom Line 52 m . These can cost up to 100,000 euros to hire for a week, but the price often includes a crew to look after the boat (including the professional skipper).

Superyacht Azimut Grande 27

The main difference from the motor boats we rent at sea is that houseboats sail on freshwater streams and are designed for exploring rivers, canals, lakes, ponds, and dams. Although houseboats generally have less powerful engines, this feature often allows them to be rented without a license in most destinations. It's important to note that these houseboats are far from mundane, offering a unique and enjoyable holiday experience on calm waters. Check out these breathtaking destinations you can explore on a houseboat.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Never been on a houseboat?  Take a look at our our guide —   First time on a houseboat: 25 things you need to know!

This is what one of the most popular houseboats, the Nicols Estivale Sixto Prestige, looks like.

Power catamaran

Recently, motor catamarans or power catamarans have become more and more popular. They combine the advantages of a catamaran (two hulls, stability, space, nets to lie on,...) while offering the speed, carefree and comfort of a motor boat. Never driven a catamaran? Check out our article — First time on a catamaran: what you need to know

Body of a two-hulled power catamaran.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Wondering what are all the types of boat you can charter? You will be surprised how many there are. Check out the article —  Boats for rent: what types of boats do charter companies offer?

How to operate a motor boat?

If you have sailing experience, driving a powerboat will seem like something very simple. You don't have to worry about ropes, sails, vignettes, masts or a flying jib. You simply start the boat and cruise wherever you want. Then it's the same as mooring with a sailboat.

One important aspect to be aware of when operating a motor boat is the  engine trim . Engine trim refers to the adjustment of the angle between the propeller and the bottom of the boat. Ideally, the propeller should be positioned vertically downward. As a motor boat gains speed, the bow of the boat may lift, causing the propeller to partially submerge. In such cases, it is the responsibility of the captain to intervene and adjust the engine trim to ensure that the propeller is aligned vertically and not at any angle other than 90 degrees to the water surface. This adjustment is crucial to prevent the boat from jumping or unnecessarily impacting the water with the bow. By maintaining the correct trim, the boat can navigate efficiently and provide a comfortable sailing experience for all on board.

YACHTING.COM TIP: Do you know how to operate the outboard motor on a dinghy? Read our article — Dinghy and outboard motor: what you need to know .

Where to sail with a motor boat?

We've selected 3 regions where you can enjoy a fantastic time with a motorboat and take advantage of its superior speed.

Vineyards and islands off Hvar

Start your journey from Split and make your way to the enchanting island of Solta or the sun-soaked Brac. For a glimpse of Croatia's renowned beaches, don't miss out on visiting Zlatni Rat. Proceed to the captivating island of Hvar, where we suggest exploring either the lively town of Hvar itself, the more serene town of Stari Grad, or the authentically charming Vrboska. Indulge in an overnight stay at a tranquil cove on the island of Ščedro, where you can delight in snorkeling alongside majestic clams. Depending on your available time and preferences, continue your voyage to the island of Vis and discover the picturesque village of Komiza, where you can experience the novelty of standing on a buoy or by the pier. During the day, take a trip to the island of Bisevo, home to the famed Blue Spila (blue cave).

Ionian Sea (and turtles!)

Rent a boat on the Greek island of Corfu. Upon taking over the boat on Saturday, take a leisurely stroll to the charming capital, Kerkyra, where you'll be enchanted by its delightful streets and atmosphere. Next, set sail south towards the island of Paxos, renowned for its breathtaking bays. During the day, make sure to indulge in a refreshing swim in Lefkada, a destination in the western part that boasts stunning beaches reminiscent of the Caribbean. Consider spending the night in the lively bay of Vasiliki, known for its vibrant nightlife and one of Greece's most famous kebab joints. The following day, continue your journey to Kefalonia and then proceed onwards to Zakynthos, famously known as the "island of turtles." If possible, sail as far south as you can towards Zakynthos, maximizing your exploration of this captivating destination.

Italian temperament

Experience the enchanting Bay of Naples, beginning in Baiae and venturing to Ischia, where you can navigate its waters at your leisure. Along the way, explore the quaint islet of Procida. Consider Ponza as an alternative to the bustling island of Capri. If time permits, visit the renowned Positano. Carry on to the breathtaking town of Amalfi, with its cliffside houses. Above all, indulge in la dolce vita.

Whether it's a motorboat or a sailboat, I'll find you the perfect choice. Give me a call.

Denisa Kliner Nguyenová

Denisa Kliner Nguyenová

Faq motor boats.

  • Dictionaries home
  • American English
  • Collocations
  • German-English
  • Grammar home
  • Practical English Usage
  • Learn & Practise Grammar (Beta)
  • Word Lists home
  • My Word Lists
  • Recent additions
  • Resources home
  • Text Checker

Definition of motorboat noun from the Oxford Advanced American Dictionary

Join our community to access the latest language learning and assessment tips from Oxford University Press!

Nearby words

Musculoskeletal Key

Fastest musculoskeletal insight engine.

  • MANUAL THERAPIST
  • MUSCULOSKELETAL MEDICINE
  • PHYSICAL MEDICINE & REHABILITATION
  • RHEUMATOLOGY
  • SPORT MEDICINE
  • Gold Membership

Basic Electromyography: Analysis of Motor Unit Action Potentials

After assessment of insertional and spontaneous activity, the needle electromyography (EMG) examination moves on to the evaluation of motor unit action potentials (MUAPs). In a process similar to the analysis of spontaneous activity, MUAPs must be assessed for morphology (duration, amplitude, phases), stability, and firing characteristics. The pattern of MUAP abnormalities that emerges from this part of the examination usually will allow a determination of whether a disorder is primarily neuropathic or myopathic and often helps determine the time course (acute vs. chronic) and severity of the lesion. The assessment of MUAPs often is demanding and improves with the experience of the electromyographer over time. The task of evaluating MUAPs is made all the more difficult by the wide variation in what is considered a normal MUAP, depending on the muscle being studied and the age of the patient. Physiology The basic component of the peripheral nervous system is the motor unit, defined as an individual motor neuron, its axon, and associated neuromuscular junctions (NMJs) and muscle fibers. The extracellular needle EMG recording of a motor unit is the MUAP ( Figure 15–1 ). The number of muscle fibers per motor unit varies greatly, from 5 to 10 in laryngeal muscles to a couple of thousand in the soleus. The transverse territory of a motor unit usually ranges from 5 to 10 mm in adults, with many motor unit territories overlapping with one another. Because of this overlap, two muscle fibers from the same motor unit rarely lie adjacent to each other. Transverse motor unit territory increases greatly with age, doubling from birth to adulthood, mostly because of the increase in individual muscle fiber size. FIGURE 15–1 The motor unit. The basic component of the peripheral nervous system is the motor unit, defined as an individual motor neuron, its axon, and associated neuromuscular junctions and muscles fibers. The extracellular needle electromyography recording of a motor unit is the motor unit action potential (MUAP). When a motor neuron depolarizes to threshold, a nerve action potential is generated and propagates down the axon. Under normal circumstances, this results in all muscle fibers of the motor unit being activated and depolarizing more or less simultaneously. Any variability between muscle fiber depolarization times is due to differences in the length of the terminal axons and in NMJ transmission times. The “size principle” governs many of the properties of motor units ( Figure 15–2 ). The size of the motor neuron is directly related to (1) the size of the axon, (2) the thickness of the myelin sheath, (3) the conduction velocity of the axon, (4) the threshold to depolarization, and (5) the metabolic type of muscle fibers that are innervated. The larger motor neurons have larger axons, with the thickest myelin sheath (hence, the fastest conduction velocity), highest threshold to depolarization, and connections to type II, fast twitch muscle fibers. Conversely, the smaller motor neurons have smaller axons, less myelin sheath, slower conduction velocity, lower threshold to depolarization, and, in general, connections to type I, slow twitch muscle fibers. Thus, with voluntary contraction, the smallest motor units with the lower thresholds fire first. As contraction increases, progressively larger motor units begin to fire. The largest type II motor units fire with maximum contraction. During routine needle EMG, most MUAPs analyzed are thus from the smaller motor units that innervate type I muscle fibers. FIGURE 15–2 Size principle and motor unit properties. During the needle EMG examination, each MUAP recorded represents the extracellular compound potential of the muscle fibers of a motor unit, weighted heavily toward the fibers nearest to the needle. A MUAP recorded just outside a muscle membrane is 1/10 to 1/100 the amplitude of the actual transmembrane potential and the amplitude decreases rapidly as the distance between the needle and the membrane increases. The classification of an MUAP as normal, neuropathic, or myopathic rests on no single finding. As is true of spontaneous activity, recorded MUAPs must be assessed for morphology (duration, polyphasia, amplitude), stability, and firing characteristics before any conclusions can be reached. Morphology MUAP properties vary widely both within and between different muscles. Even within a muscle, there is a wide range of normal motor unit morphology, with MUAP size following a bell-shaped distribution curve ( Figure 15–3 ). Due to this normal variability, normal values of MUAP morphology are based on the mean of many different MUAPs. The analysis of MUAP morphology can be performed on either a qualitative or a quantitative basis. To perform quantitative MUAP analysis, one must isolate 20 different MUAPs for each muscle being studied and measure their individual durations, amplitudes, and number of phases. From these values, the mean duration, amplitude, and number of phases are calculated and compared with a set of normal values for that particular muscle and age group. MUAP morphology varies depending on the muscle being studied and the patient’s age. This is particularly true of MUAP duration ( Table 15–1 ). In general, MUAPs in proximal muscles tend to be shorter in duration than those in more distal muscles. MUAP size in adults is larger than in children, primarily because of an increase in the size of muscle fibers during development. In addition, MUAP size is generally larger in older individuals, probably as the result of dropout of motor units from the normal effects of aging, leading to some compensatory “normal” reinnervation. The loss of motor units has been estimated to be approximately 1% per year, beginning in the third decade of life, which then increases rapidly after age 60. FIGURE 15–3 Range of normal motor unit action potential (MUAP) duration and amplitude. Histogram of MUAP duration and amplitude in the biceps brachii of a normal subject. Note that both MUAP duration and amplitude vary markedly in normal muscles, with small and large units in the same muscle. MUAP duration or amplitude should not be classified as abnormal based on one or two MUAPs but requires a mean of many motor units. (Reprinted with permission from Buchthal, F., Guld, C., Rosenfalck, P., 1954. Action potential parameters in normal human muscle and their dependence on physical variables. Acta Physiol Scand 32, 200.) Table 15–1 Mean Motor Unit Action Potential Duration Based on Age and Muscle Group Age of Subjects Arm Muscles Leg Muscles Deltoid Biceps Triceps Thenar ADM Quad, BF Gastroc Tib Ant Per Long EDB Facial 0–4 7.9–10.1 6.4–8.2 7.2–9.3 7.1–9.1 8.3–10.6 7.2–9.2 6.4–8.2 8.0–10.2 6.8–7.4 6.3–8.1 3.7–4.7 5–9 8.0–10.8 6.5–8.8 7.3–9.9 7.2–9.8 8.4–11.4 7.3–9.9 6.5–8.8 8.1–11.0 5.9–7.9 6.4–8.7 3.8–5.1 10–14 8.1–11.2 6.6–9.1 7.5–10.3 7.3–10.1 8.5–11.7 7.4–10.2 6.6–9.1 8.2–11.3 5.9–8.2 6.5–9.0 3.9–5.3 15–19 8.6–12.2 7.0–9.9 7.9–11.2 7.8–11.0 9.0–12.8 7.8–11.1 7.0–9.9 8.7–12.3 6.3–8.9 6.9–9.8 4.1–5.7 20–29 9.5–13.2 7.7–10.7 8.7–12.1 8.5–11.9 9.9–13.8 8.6–12.0 7.7–10.7 9.6–13.3 6.9–9.6 7.6–10.6 4.4–6.2 30–39 11.1–14.9 9.0–12.1 10.2–13.7 10.0–13.4 11.6–15.6 10.1–13.5 9.0–12.1 11.2–15.1 8.1–10.9 8.9–12.0 5.2–7.1 40–49 11.8–15.7 9.6–12.8 10.9–14.5 10.7–14.2 12.4–16.5 10.7–14.3 9.6–12.8 11.9–15.9 8.6–11.5 9.5–12.7 5.6–7.4 50–59 12.8–16.7 10.4–13.6 11.8–15.4 11.5–15.1 13.4–17.5 11.6–15.2 10.4–13.6 12.9–16.9 9.4–12.2 10.3–13.5 6.0–7.9 60–69 13.3–17.3 10.8–14.1 12.2–15.9 12.0–15.7 13.9–18.2 12.1–15.8 10.8–14.1 13.4–17.5 9.7–12.7 10.7–14.0 6.3–8.2 70–79 13.7–17.7 11.1–14.4 12.5–16.3 12.3–16.0 14.3–18.6 12.4–16.1 11.1–14.4 13.8–17.9 10.0–13.0 11.0–14.3 6.5–8.3   ADM, abductor digiti minimi; BF, biceps femoris; EDB, extensor digitorum brevis; Gastroc, gastrocnemius; Per Long, peroneus longus; Quad, quadriceps; Tib Ant, tibialis anterior. Reprinted with permission from Buchthal, F., Rosenfalck, P. Action potential parameters in different human muscles. Acta Psychiatr Neurol Scand, © 1955 Munsgaard International Publishers Ltd, Copenhagen, Denmark. Only by comparing mean MUAP morphology in each muscle studied to normal values for that particular muscle and age group can one determine whether the morphology is truly abnormal. Previously, quantitative MUAP analysis was tedious and time consuming. However, many modern EMG machines now have programs that largely automate the procedure. With experience over time, however, the well-trained electromyographer usually can perform qualitative MUAP assessment with the same precision as can be achieved using quantitative methods. Essentially the same procedure is used. The needle is moved to several locations within the muscle until approximately 20 different MUAPs have been examined, qualitatively analyzed, and compared to the expected normal values for that particular muscle and age group. Duration MUAP duration is the parameter that best reflects the number of muscle fibers within a motor unit ( Figure 15–4 ). Typical MUAP duration is between 5 and 15 ms. Duration is defined as the time from the initial deflection from baseline to the final return of the MUAP to baseline. It depends primarily on the number of muscle fibers within the motor unit and the dispersion of their depolarizations over time. Dispersion in turn depends on the longitudinal and transverse scatter of endplates and on variations in terminal distances and conduction velocities. Duration lengthens as the number of fibers and the territory of a motor unit increase; it varies directly with age (increased age, increased duration) and inversely with temperature (decreased temperature, increased duration) and depends on the individual muscle being studied. Proximal and bulbofacial muscles in general have MUAPs of shorter duration. When performing EMG, it often is more rewarding to listen to the potential than to see it. This is especially true when evaluating MUAP duration, because duration correlates with pitch . Long-duration MUAPs (low frequencies) sound dull and thuddy, whereas short-duration MUAPs (higher frequencies) sound crisp and static-like. As the electromyographer gains experience, the sound of a long-duration versus a short-duration MUAP becomes unmistakable. FIGURE 15–4 Motor unit action potential (MUAP) measurements. Duration is measured as the time from the initial deflection of the MUAP from baseline to its final return to baseline. It is the parameter that best reflects the number of muscle fibers in the motor unit. Amplitude reflects only muscle fibers very close to the needle and is measured peak to peak. Phases (shaded areas) can be determined by counting the number of baseline crossings and adding one. MUAPs are generally triphasic. Serrations (also called turns) are changes in direction of the potential that do not cross the baseline. The major spike is the largest positive-to-negative deflection, usually occurring after the first positive peak. Satellite, or linked, potentials occur after the main potential and usually represent early reinnervation of muscle fibers. Polyphasia, Serrations, and Satellite Potentials Polyphasia is a measure of synchrony, that is, the extent to which the muscle fibers within a motor unit fire more or less at the same time. This is a nonspecific measure and may be abnormal in both myopathic and neuropathic disorders. The number of phases can be easily calculated by counting the number of baseline crossings of the MUAP and adding one ( Figure 15–4 ). Normally, MUAPs have two to four phases. However, increased polyphasia may be seen in up to 5 to 10% of the MUAPs in any muscle and is considered normal. The one exception is the deltoid, where up to 25% polyphasia may be normal. Increased polyphasia beyond 10% in most muscles and 25% in the deltoid is always abnormal. Through the speaker, polyphasic MUAPs are recognized as a high-frequency “clicking” sound. Serrations (also called turns) are defined as changes in the direction of the potential that do not cross the baseline. Increased polyphasia and serrations have similar implications, indicating less synchronous firing of muscle fibers within a motor unit. Often, a serration can be changed into an additional phase with needle movement. Satellite potentials (also known as linked potentials or parasite potentials ) are interesting phenomena seen in early reinnervation. After denervation, muscle fibers often are reinnervated by collateral sprouts from adjacent intact motor units. The newly formed sprout often is small, unmyelinated or thinly myelinated, and therefore very slowly conducting. Because of the slow conduction time and increased distance, reinnervated muscle fibers are seen as time-locked potentials that trail the main MUAP ( Figures 15–5 and 15–6 ). These satellite potentials are extremely unstable (see section on Stability ) and may vary slightly in their firing rate or may block and not fire at all ( Figure 15–7 ). Over time, the sprout matures, and the thickness of the myelin and consequently the conduction velocity increase. The satellite potential then fires more closely to the main potential and ultimately will become an additional phase or serration within the main complex. It is usually necessary to put the main MUAP on a delay line to appreciate a satellite potential and to demonstrate that it is time locked to the main potential. FIGURE 15–5 Collateral sprouting and satellite potentials. A: Normal state. B: Following partial denervation, the injured axon(s) undergoes wallerian degeneration. C: Reinnervation commonly occurs from sprouting by adjacent surviving axons. In early reinnervation, sprouts are small and thinly myelinated and conduct slowly. Because of the slow conduction time and increased distance, these reinnervated fibers initially occur as time-locked potentials (satellite potentials) trailing the main motor unit action potential (MUAP). As sprouts mature and conduct more quickly, the time-locked potentials are eventually incorporated into the main MUAP, resulting in an MUAP with increased amplitude, duration, and number of phases.

Share this:

  • Click to share on Twitter (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Facebook (Opens in new window)

Related posts:

motorboat action meaning

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Comments are closed for this page.

motorboat action meaning

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

motorboat action meaning

IMAGES

  1. Action by stand on vessel

    motorboat action meaning

  2. Motorboat

    motorboat action meaning

  3. A Motorboat is Crossing Paths with a PWC. What Action Should be Taken?

    motorboat action meaning

  4. A Motorboat Is Crossing Paths With A PWC. What Action To Take?

    motorboat action meaning

  5. The Best Activities To Do On A Motorboat

    motorboat action meaning

  6. Motorboat

    motorboat action meaning

VIDEO

  1. RC-Boot RAPSCALLION BL RTR (500108010) ENGLISH Version

  2. Barnegat Lighthouse Boat Action Video #4

  3. Deepwater Motor Boat Club

  4. Flying a Kitewing on Whitmore Lake

  5. true meaning of motorboat #stitch #virlshort #funny

  6. Powerboat vs Cruise Ship! Midnight Express

COMMENTS

  1. What is the meaning of the term 'motorboating' and what is its ...

    The first meaning refers to the activity of traveling in a motorboat [2]. This is the literal definition of the term and is commonly used in the context of recreational boating or water sports. The second meaning, which is more commonly known as slang, refers to a sexual act.

  2. Full Speed Ahead: The Origins of 'Motorboating'

    The Maritime Connection First and foremost, let's get one thing straight - yes, there is an actual connection between motorboating and the watercraft. As you may know, motorboats create a particular sound and motion when they're speeding through water. The term "motorboating," in its colloquial usage, is derived from the mimicking of this motion and sound in a rather intimate context - usually ...

  3. Motor Action

    Motor act indicates a series of movements organized in such a way as to achieve a specific motor goal (e.g., grasping an object). Motor action is a sequence of motor acts that allows individuals to achieve, as their final outcome, a behavioral goal, endowed with a positive or negative reward.

  4. motorboating

    motorboating. motorboating, sport of navigating a motor-powered vessel on the water. It is done on either fresh- or saltwater and may be competitive or recreational. The first successful motorboat traveled (1887) a few yards on the Seine River in Paris. As the internal-combustion engine was improved, the motorboat became a practical means of ...

  5. Motorboat

    A motorboat, speedboat or powerboat is a boat that is exclusively powered by an engine. Some motorboats are fitted with inboard engines, others have an outboard motor installed on the rear, containing the internal combustion engine, the gearbox and the propeller in one portable unit. An inboard-outboard contains a hybrid of an inboard and an ...

  6. Electric motor

    An electric motor is a machine that converts electrical energy into mechanical energy. Most electric motors operate through the interaction between the motor's magnetic field and electric current in a wire winding to generate force in the form of torque applied on the motor's shaft. An electric generator is mechanically identical to an electric ...

  7. How Electric Motors and Generators Work

    Electric cars hybrids rely upon electric motors for propulsion and assistance. Here is a look at how these motors and generators work to generate power.

  8. Ideomotor phenomenon

    The ideomotor phenomenon is a psychological phenomenon wherein a subject makes motions unconsciously. Also called ideomotor response (or ideomotor reflex) and abbreviated to IMR, it is a concept in hypnosis and psychological research. [2] It is derived from the terms "ideo" (idea, or mental representation) and "motor" (muscular action).

  9. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and ...

    By action understanding, we mean the capacity to achieve the internal description of an action and to use it to organize appropriate future behaviour.

  10. Apraxia

    Apraxia is a neurological disorder that affects motor cognition, planning, and task performance without apparent neurological insult to basic motor function, sensation, or comprehension. This disorder can result from various types of brain lesions, including stroke, dementia, tumors, neurocognitive disorders, and brain injuries. Identifying and diagnosing apraxia primarily relies on clinical ...

  11. First time on a motor boat: what you should know

    Motor boats don't often take centre stage in our magazine, but we're about to change that. This in-depth feature explores the different kinds of motorboats, their manufacturers, how they differ from sailboats, and weighs their pros and cons. We'll help you figure out if a motorboat is the right fit for you, when to best venture out on one, and we'll delve into the requirements and conditions ...

  12. PDF Microsoft Word

    Additionally, these data show that congruity effects between motor action and word meaning extend beyond language comprehension to language production. Furthermore, influences of language and action are bidirectional: using language with literal or metaphorical spatial content influences the execution of simple, irrelevant motor actions - and ...

  13. Motorboat

    motorboat, a relatively small watercraft propelled by an internal-combustion or electric engine. Motorboats range in size from miniature craft designed to carry one person to seagoing vessels of 100 feet (30 m) or more. Most motorboats, however, have space for six passengers or fewer. Motorboats are used recreationally for traveling on water ...

  14. Motor cognition-motor semantics: Action perception theory of cognition

    The implication is that the link between abstract emotional meaning and utterances and symbols is by way of the manifestation of emotion in action, that is by way of information in the motor system.

  15. Motor cognition

    Motor cognition. The concept of motor cognition grasps the notion that cognition is embodied in action, and that the motor system participates in what is usually considered as mental processing, including those involved in social interaction. [1] The fundamental unit of the motor cognition paradigm is action, defined as the movements produced ...

  16. motorboat

    motorboat meaning, definition, what is motorboat: a small fast boat with an engine: Learn more.

  17. motorboat noun

    Definition of motorboat noun in Oxford Advanced American Dictionary. Meaning, pronunciation, picture, example sentences, grammar, usage notes, synonyms and more.

  18. Basic Electromyography: Analysis of Motor Unit Action Potentials

    The basic component of the peripheral nervous system is the motor unit, defined as an individual motor neuron, its axon, and associated neuromuscular junctions and muscles fibers. The extracellular needle electromyography recording of a motor unit is the motor unit action potential (MUAP). When a motor neuron depolarizes to threshold, a nerve ...

  19. Motor planning

    Motor planning. In psychology and neuroscience, motor planning is a set of processes related to the preparation of a movement that occurs during the reaction time (the time between the presentation of a stimulus to a person and that person's initiation of a motor response). Colloquially, the term applies to any process involved in the ...